Richard G. Petty, MD

Knowing Woman

Irene Claremont de Castillejo.jpg

“A woman today lives in perpetual conflict. She cannot slay the dragon of the unconscious without severing her own essential contact with it; without in fact destroying her feminine strength and becoming a mere pseudo-man. Her task is a peculiarly difficult one. She needs the focused consciousness her animus alone can give her, yet she must not forsake her woman?s role of mediator to man. Through a woman, man finds his soul. She must never forget this. Through a woman, not through a pseudo-man. Through man, woman finds the animus who can express the soul she has never lost. Her burning need is to trust her own diffuse awareness, to know what she knows and to learn to speak of it, for until it is expressed she does not wholly know it.”

–Irene Claremont de Castillejo English-born Jungian Analyst who Specialized in “Feminine Psychology,” and Worked with Emma Jung and Toni Wolff, 1885-1967)


“Knowing Woman: A Feminine Psychology” (Irene de Castillejo)

Men, Women and Forgiveness

We recently discussed the importance of forgiveness on health. Over the years studies have shown that men tend to be more vengeful than women, presumably because they have been taught from childhood to empathize with others and build relationships. Though there could yet be a biological basis for this difference.

New data from Case Western Reserve University, Florida State University, Arizona State and Hope College published in the Journal of Personality and Social Psychology suggests that forgiveness does not come naturally for both sexes. Psychologist Julie Juola Exline’s research indicates that men have a harder time forgiving than women do. However that can change if men develop empathy toward an offender by seeing they may also be capable of similar actions. That empathy closes the gender gap and men become less vengeful.

The authors conducted seven forgiveness-related studies (1,2,3,4,5,6,7) between 1998 and 2005 that involved more than1,400 college students: Gender differences have been a robust finding.

The studies used hypothetical situations, actual recalled offenses, individual and group situations and surveys to study the ability to forgive. When men were asked to recall offenses they had committed themselves, they became less vengeful toward people who had offended them. Women started at a lower baseline for vengeance, but thinking about their own transgressions had no effect on levels of unforgiving. When women were asked to recall a similar offense in relation to the other’s offense, women felt guilty and tended to magnify the other’s offense.

The researchers found that people of both genders are more forgiving when they see themselves as capable of committing a similar action; it tends to make the offense seem smaller and increases empathic understanding of the offense. Therefore people similar to the offenders and therefore more forgiving attitudes.

The ability to identify with the offender and forgive also happens in intergroup conflicts. In a study on forgiveness of the 9/11 terrorists Exline comments that,

“When people could envision their own government committing acts similar to those of the terrorists, they were less vengeful. For example, they were less likely to believe that perpetrators should be killed on the spot or given the death penalty, and they were more supportive of negotiations and economic aid.”

It is not difficult to see that prosecution and defense attorneys are going to study this data carefully. It will likely come into play during jury selection processes.

And I am going to think about it the next time that I am called upon to serve on a medical school interview board.

“An eye for an eye will only serve to make the whole world blind.”
–Mahatma Gandhi (Indian Nationalist and World Teacher, 1869-1948)

“And be ye kind one to another, tenderhearted, forgiving one another, even as God, for Christ’s sake, hath forgiven you.”
–The Bible (Ephesians 4:32)

“Everyone and everything I see will lean toward me and bless me. I will recognize in everyone my dearest friend. What could there be to fear in a world that I have forgiven, that has forgiven me?”
–A Course in Miracles (Book of Spiritual Principles Scribed by Dr. Helen Schucman between 1965 and 1975, and First Published in 1976)

Blondes Make Us Dumb??

No, this isn’t just a cheap excuse to display a picture of Pamela Anderson.

I just received this from a regular reader:

“Dear Dr Petty,

Have you seen this article Times Online?

“WHEN men meet fair-haired women they really do have a “blonde moment”. Scientists have found that their mental performance drops, apparently because they believe they are dealing with someone less intelligent.

Researchers discovered what might be called the “bimbo delusion” by studying men’s ability to complete general knowledge tests after exposure to different women. The academics found that men’s scores fell after they were shown pictures of blondes.

Further analysis convinced the team that, rather than simply being distracted by the flaxen hair, those who performed poorly had been unconsciously driven by social stereotypes to “think blonde”.

“This proves that people confronted with stereotypes generally behave in line with them,” said Thierry Meyer, joint author of the study and professor of social psychology at the University of Paris X-Nanterre. “In this case blondes have the potential to make people act in a dumber way, because they mimic the unconscious stereotype of the dumb blonde.”

Do you have any comment to make about the research??”

The answer is that I have seen this article all over the Internet, with all kinds of sage comments.

The trouble is that I have not yet been able to read and critique the research. As far as I know, neither the hard copy nor electronic versions are available yet, so I cannot evaluate the report.

This highlights one of the problems of the Internet: news travels across it like the wind, yet a lot of material is passed on without analysis. So the story is fun to read, but until we can see and analyze the data, we cannot comment.

As soon as I get a copy I shall see if I have anything to add to the firestorm of commentary!

Marriage Makes You…

A Chinese sage once said that if a woman took a pea and placed it in a large jar every time that she had sex in the first year of marriage, and then took out a pea every time that she had sex in the rest of her life, the jar would never be empty.

So many couples seem to think that a declining interest in sex I something to be expected, and agony columns and websites are crammed with complaints. Does it have to be that way? Is it just that people get bored with each other, or are too tired to bother?

Some intriguing new research suggests that there may also be a physical factor, reporting lower testosterone levels among married men compared with single, unmarried men.

The research is reported in this month’s issue of Current Anthropology by Peter B. Gray from the University of Nevada, Las Vegas, Peter T. Ellison from Harvard University and Benjamin C. Campbell from Boston University.

They investigated the links between male testosterone levels and marital status among modern-day cattle farmers from the Ariaal tribe in northern Kenya. Less than 1.5 percent of these men consider that their wives are a source of emotional support. The Ariaal males serve as herd boys until they reach puberty. They then undergo initiation, become warriors and begin to accumulate livestock. They do not marry and have children until around age 30. They value social bonds with male peers more than spousal or familial bonds.

The researchers measured testosterone in morning and afternoon saliva samples of more than 200 Ariaal men over the age of twenty. They found that monogamously married men had lower testosterone levels than unmarried men in both the morning and afternoon. However polygynously married men with more than one wife had even lower levels of testosterone than the monogamously married men.

The data suggest that male testosterone levels might reflect variations in male mating efforts. It may also be that in the tribal setting, older men, who typically have lower testosterone levels, may have the social status and wealth to obtain more than one wife.

Given the increasing amount of data concerning the importance of testosterone for normal brain and arterial function, this may be a big wakeup call. If men do nothing once they are in a relationship, and their brains begin to think that the “hunt” is over, their testosterone levels can plummet.

And that can be fatal.

It is essential for couples to work to keep passion – and testosterone – alive.

It is not sexism, it is millions of years of evolution.

Sex, Handedness and the Brain

I have commented before on my longstanding interest in handedness and laterality, as well as gender differences in the brain. A recurrent question when we look at both of them is whether culture has any role in the development of either. Are some of the differences in male and female brains driven by educational opportunities and cultural expectations, or is there something innate about them?

A recently published paper in PLoS ONE reports finding both sex and handedness influences on the relative size of the corpus callosum in Capuchin monkeys.

Capuchin monkeys are playful, inquisitive primates best known as the “Organ Grinder” monkeys. They have great manual dexterity, complex social behavior, and cognitive abilities. The new research now shows that just like humans, they display a fundamental sex difference in the organization of the brain, specifically in the corpus callosum, the band of white matter that connects the two hemispheres of the brain.

In the study, thirteen adult capuchins underwent magnetic resonance imaging of the brain to determine the size of their corpus callosum. The monkeys were later given a task to determine hand preference. The authors’ results led them to conclude that, as in humans, male capuchins have a smaller relative size of the corpus callosum than females, and right-handed individuals have a smaller relative size of the corpus callosum than left-handed individuals.

As the two hemispheres show greater independence of function, the relative size of the corpus callosum is expected to be smaller. This has been documented in humans, and same pattern was found in capuchins.

This finding may be related to hemispheric specialization for complex foraging tasks that require the integration of motor actions and visuospatial information. In the wild, capuchin monkeys live in trees as well as on the ground, and they are known to be very good at capturing small swift prey such as birds, lizards, and squirrels.

Men, Exercise and Broken Bones

One of the problems that we all face as we get older is that unless we are careful, our bones can gradually become weaker. This is a particular problem in women, but a growing body of research indicates that men can run into trouble as well. At one end of the spectrum is “osteopenia:” a reduction in bone mineral density, and at the other end of the spectrum is full-blown osteoporosis.

The reason for taking the problem so seriously is that any degree of thinning of the bones can predispose us to so-called “osteoporotic fractures.” Sad to say, serious fractures are common among older people and can have devastating consequences, particularly if a hip is broken. Man people find it difficult to walk again. For years now we have recommended that women should take exercise to reduce the risk of thinning of their bones, particularly after menopause. But now research published in PLoS Medicine indicates that men who participate in sport or other vigorous activity may also reduce their risk of fractures.

Karl Michaelsson and colleagues at University Hospital, Uppsala, Sweden, report research in which over 2000 men who were initially studied in 1970-1973. The amount of physical activity that they took outside working hours was recorded, and then they were questioned again as they aged. Each of the surviving men was questioned again when they were aged 60, 70, 77, and 82. About half of the men were still alive at the end of the study, and the researchers recorded the number of fractures that the men had suffered during the 35 years of the study.

Based on the answers to the questions on physical activity at the start of the study, the researchers divided the men into three groups: those whose lifestyle was considered to be ”sedentary,” those whose leisure activities included some walking and cycling and those who participated in sports for at least 3 hours a week. These were referred to as the low, medium, and high-activity groups. Over the 35 years, 428 men had at least one fracture and 134 broke a hip. However, there were significant differences between the groups. 20% of the low-activity men had fractures, compared with 13% of those with medium activity and only 8% of those in the high-activity group. Of greatest importance was that the chance of having a hip fracture – the worst type of broken bone in an older person – was dramatically reduced by increased activity.

The researchers conclude that taking exercise reduces the risk of osteoporotic fracture in men. Participating in sports seems to be particularly effective: they calculated that one-third of fractures could be prevented if men could be persuaded to take part in sports regularly.

There is also an excellent summary by Harri Sievänen and Pekka Kannus from Tampere in Finland. As always each entire article is available online.

And I am to get my running shoes this very instant…


Hormones, Addictions and Mood

People working with mental illness have been for years now been puzzled by two observations. The first is that mood disorders and schizophrenia follow quite different trajectories in men and women. Women tend to be more vulnerable to mood disorders and if they get schizophrenia it tends to be less severe and to have fewer “negative” symptoms, such as flat, blunted or constricted affect and emotion, poverty of speech and lack of motivation until after menopause. We have looked at some of the reasons for the different rates of mood disorder, in terms of relationships and social pressures, but there must also be a biological component. The second puzzle is that women are more vulnerable to addictive drugs in the days before they ovulate.

New research published in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences may provide part of the answer to both puzzles.

Colleagues at the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), a component of the National Institutes of Health (NIH), have conducted a fascinating imaging study that has shown that fluctuations in levels of sex hormones during women’s menstrual cycles affect the responsiveness of the reward systems in the brain.

The reward system circuits include the:

  • Prefrontal cortex, which has key roles in thinking, planning and in the control of our emotions and impulses
  • Amygdala, which is involved in rapid and intense emotional reactions and the formation of emotional memories
  • Hippocampus, which is involved in learning, memory and navigation
  • Striatum that relays signals from these areas to the cerebral cortex

It has been known for some time that neurons in the reward circuits are rich in estrogen and progesterone receptors. However, how these hormones influence reward circuit activity in humans has remained unclear.

The researchers used functional magnetic resonance (fMRI) imaging to examine brain activity of 13 women and 13 men while they performed a task that involved simulated slot machines. The women were scanned while they did the task, both before and after ovulation.

When anticipating a reward, in the pre-ovulation phase of their menstrual cycles the women showed more activity in the amygdala and frontal cortex. When women were actually winning prizes, their reward systems were more active if they were in the phase of their menstrual cycle preceding ovulation. This phase of the cycle is dominated by estrogen, compared to postovulatory phase when estrogen and progesterone are both present. When winning, the main systems that became active were in the parts of the brain involved in pleasure and reward.

The researchers also demonstrated that the reward-related brain activity was directly linked to levels of sex hormones. Activity in the amygdala and hippocampus was in directly linked to estrogen levels, regardless of where a woman was in her cycle. When women won prizes during the post-ovulatory phase of the cycle, progesterone modulated the effect of estrogen on the reward circuit.

Men showed a different activation profile from women during both anticipation and delivery of rewards. Men had more activity in the striatum during anticipation compared with women. On the other hand, women had more activity in a frontal cortex when they won prizes.

This research could have a number of important implications. The most obvious is that it confirms what many women know already: they are more likely to take addictive substances or to engage in pleasurable – but perhaps impulsive or risky – behaviors just before they ovulate.

It is not difficult to imagine why this might have developed during evolution.

“Coming to terms with the rhythms of women’s lives means coming to terms with life itself, accepting the imperatives of the body rather than the imperatives of an artificial, man-made, perhaps transcendentally beautiful civilization. Emphasis on the male work-rhythm is an emphasis on infinite possibilities; emphasis on the female rhythms is an emphasis on a defined pattern, on limitation.”
–Margaret Mead (American Anthropologist and Writer, 1901-1978)

Toxoplasmosis and Behavior

Last August I wrote an article about some extraordinary new evidence implicating Toxoplasma gondii in some psychological and psychiatric illnesses. Latent infection with
Toxoplasma gondii is amongst the most prevalent of human infections and it
had been generally assumed that it is asymptomatic unless there is
congenital transmission or reactivation because a person has an immune system that has become depressed or compromised. That assumption is being
completely re-evaluated

The article generated some extremely interesting correspondence and some spirited discussions.

Here is a very insightful letter from a physician:

Dear Dr. Petty,

I thought about the concept of psychological illness caused by a virus or other organism. I was wondering what would be the mode of dispersion of such a virus. Upper respiratory tract infections, skin and gastrointestinal infections spread by cough, by touch and hand to mouth respectively. How would such a brain virus or protozoal organism promote itself? Of course it could be by the above methods but it seems that there should some way that the specific disease process is connected to a behavior that helps it to spread itself. 

Then I got to thinking; diseases have learned physical ways to disseminate themselves, I wonder if a disease could change behavior to promote it’s own dissemination and survival? I imagine that if that were true, people with the flu would be sociable, people with infectious diarrhea would be sociable and hungry, people with AIDS would have increased libido. I haven’t yet seen any data for this. Although I’ve always felt that there was one disease that did alter behavior in a way that is conducive to disseminating itself, and that is rabies. The host goes from being docile, to seeing all others as the enemy. He then attacks them, bites them and thus passes on the organism. A true mind altering virus, although it’s psychology works better with animals than with people. Do you think that there are other diseases that spread purely by behavior, that cause the host to seek out the next host and not just pass the disease from one to another just due to proximity?


This was my response:

What great questions!

And believe it or not, there’s quite a lot of empirical research on these very topics.

There is a whole textbook on the behavioral effects of parasites edited by Janice Moore entitled  Parasites and the Behavior of Animals. Here’s an interesting one: rats and mice are hard wired to avoid cats. Millions of years of programming have ensured that Tom’s very presence would send Jerry packing. Cats carry Toxoplasma gondii and if mice or rats become infected with it, usually by eating cat poop, they lose their fear of felines. So now Tom can have lunch at his leisure.

I’ve also talked about the way in which people with creativity and schizotypal personality disorder (i.e. carriers of genetic risk) tend to be promiscuous, while people with schizophrenia have fewer children. Both groups tend to get more sexually transmitted diseases than the general population. It would be tempting to think that toxoplasmosis can be spread that way, however there’s a 32-year old study in German that showed that Toxoplasma was not transmitted by intercourse. However, cytomegalovirus, a common partner to Toxoplasma may be. And both modulate dopamine activity in the regions of the brain involved in salience.

I have done a very detailed literature search encompassing papers written in all the languages that I can read, and have not been able to find any clear evidence of behavior change induced by HIV, influenza or infectious diarrhea: what interesting and important questions to research.

We do have some more data confirming the effects of Toxoplasma infections on the behavior of rats: they become less anxious and therefore do not respond to environmental threats as quickly as uninfected rodents. An antipsychotic medication (haloperidol), a mood stabilizer (valproic acid) and two chemotherapeutic agents – pyrimethamine or Dapsone – have all been shown to prevent the development of Toxoplasma-induced behavioral change.

Another recent study from the Departments of Parasitology, Microbiology and Zoology, Charles University, the Centre of Reproductive Medicine and GynCentrum, in the Czech Republic also speaks to the significance of latent Toxoplasma infections: the presence of the parasite in the blood of pregnant women increases their chance of giving birth to boys. The increased survival of male embryos in infected women may be explained by Toxoplasmosis infections modulating and suppressing the immune system.

If Toxoplasma plays a part in the development of some psychiatric illnesses, yet a high proportion of the population carries it without any problems, one obvious question is what activates it? Environmental stress might, perhaps, cause the Toxoplasma to become reactivated and play a part in the development of specific psychiatric symptoms.

This story is continuing to develop and I am going to watch it closely. If it is confirmed, it could open up some brand new avenues for helping treat and perhaps even prevent some types of psychiatric illness.

Testosterone and the Death of Brain Cells

I’m sure that you’ve heard the Robin Williams joke, “See, the problem is that God gives men a brain and a penis, and only enough blood to run one at a time.”

Well it may turn out that Robin was right for the wrong reason.

Typically thought of as the “male hormone,” testosterone plays key roles in maintaining health and wellness in both men and women. It is true that most men produce about twenty ties as much testosterone as women, but in both sexes, it is involved in energy, libido, and immune function and helps protect against osteoporosis. It is also essential for the normal development, growth and functioning of the brain. In small amounts it may also be neuroprotective.

However, too much of a good thing can quickly turn bad. Researchers from the Departments of Pharmacology and Cellular and Molecular Physiology at Yale University in New Haven, Connecticut have just published an important study of apoptosis or programmed cell death in neurons exposed to excessive amounts of testosterone. Apoptosis is a process for disposing of un-needed or unwanted cells, but if it gets out of control, it can begin to remove cells that should have been left alone. Apoptosis is thought to pay a role in illnesses including Alzheimer’s disease and schizophrenia.

While too much testosterone destroyed nerve cells, estrogen appeared to be neuroprotective: there was less cell death in the presence of the hormone.

This new finding has a number of important practical implications.

Testosterone is one of the hormones abused by some athletes. It certainly can enable them to pump up their muscles, but it may also make them aggressive. Now we know that the practice may also kill neurons. And loss of brain cells is associated with a loss of brain function. This is yet another reason why people should think long and hard before they try to use testosterone supplements. The concentrations used in the experiments were very close to what we might expect to see in someone supplementing with the hormone.

These effects of testosterone on neurons will likely have long term effects on brain function. Though you do generate new connections and some new neurons throughout life, there is a limit to how many you can put back, once they’ve been tainted by testosterone.

And since this is election year here in the United States, I’m sure that we’re now going to have to have a string of off-color jokes about the esteemed Governor of California….

Women, Asthma and the Brain

There’s been a longstanding puzzle in medicine. Well actually there are lots of them, but here’s one that may be a puzzle no more.

For many years now, it’s been known that asthma is more common in women, and also that psychological stress can cause flare ups of asthma.

Many women experience “menstrual flaring:” a worsening of asthma around the time of their menstrual period. There is also a strange paradox: some women with asthma wheeze less if they take an oral contraceptive, while some non-asthmatic women begin to wheeze when they take it. In some women pregnancy makes asthma worse, and in others it affords months of relief of symptoms. Women who are obese are more likely to get asthma, presumably because their intra-abdominal fat stores are churning out inflammatory mediators.

Researchers from the University of Wisconsin have shed  some important light on this link between asthma and the brain. In research published in the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. In the study, six patients with mild asthma were exposed to ragweed or dust-mite extracts. The subjects were shown three different categories of words: asthma-related (e.g., "wheeze"), non-asthma negative ("loneliness") or neutral ("curtains").

Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, they showed that activity in two regions, known as the anterior cingulate cortex and the insula showed increased activity when the asthma-related words were heard compared with the other types. What is more, this enhanced activity was specifically linked to physiologic signals from the ragweed and dust-mite extracts. So being exposed to asthma-relevant emotional stimuli is associated with markers of inflammation and airway obstruction in asthmatic people exposed to an asthma-producing antigen.

In people with asthma and other stress-related conditions, these brain regions may be hyper-responsive to disease-specific emotional and physiologic signals. Taken together, these could contribute to problems that worsen the asthma, such as inflammation.

And one of the ways of making these regions of the brain hyper-responsive? Bathe them in estrogen.

That still does not explain why pregnancy and the oral contraceptive makes some women’s asthma better, and does the opposite in others. But it may just have to do with the “set point” of the cells in these regions of the brain. In the same way that we might set the thermostat in out house. An already hyper-responsive brain might be normalized and an under-active one stimulated to be over-active.

We need to do some more experiments, but these are a great start.

If you ever wheeze, have a look to see if there are stressors or hormonal events that trigger you. Whether you are being treated with homeopathy, herbals or conventional therapy, knowing when to expect trouble gives you the power to adapt you treatment when you are entering a risky time in your life.

logo logo logo logo logo logo